The
Placebo Effect
The human brain is a phenomenal muscle.
It is the root of every individual and how each person reacts to not only the
people around them but also how humans react to themselves both mentally and
physically. The brain has the ability to protect people when they are scared,
forget memories that are hurtful and even cure them when they are sick. So if
the human brain is so powerful why do placebos work for some people and not
others? Are placebos an effective treatment and is there any evidence that they
actually work? When is it appropriate to use a placebo in lieu of a medication
that works? Doctors have been trying to
come to an agreement for years now on whether or not placebos are useful. One
of the biggest concerns is there is no way to control a placebo in a clinical
trial.
There are many factors involved when it
comes to studying the effects of a placebo. First is the patient a good
candidate and open to the idea of a placebo? How can the FDA control placebos
when there are no guidelines. For example the ingredients in a placebo are
rarely disclosed so each type of placebo is very different. Is there a way to
create a control?
The biggest obstacle for doctors and
scientists is finding a good candidate for placebos. Because so much of what
humans can do and overcome is compelled by the human brain it is nearly
impossible to test placebos. Author Bagley from Newsweek wrote an article called Placebo Nation: Just Believe. In
it he compares placebos to the magic feather of Dumbo the flying elephant
(Owens 817). Dumbo didn’t need the feather to fly it simply gave him the
confidence to do it. Medication can be very similar. There are many times when
the public doesn’t need medicine to fight off an illness just something to
trick the body into feeling better. Instead of being happy that our bodies can
fight off illness completely on its own the majority of people are outraged
when they discover that the medicine prescribed to them was nothing short of
expensive magic feathers.
New studies have shown that placebos are
physiologically measurable with condition specific pathways. With these new
studies also comes new testing. In the past doctors conducting a clinical trial
that involves placebos would have the daunting task of finding the right
candidate for placebo testing. This effort was largely abandoned because of
inconsistent results. “The widespread conclusion was there was no reliable
individual difference factor at work in placebo responding that has been a
major stumbling black for placebo theorist for decades,” (Owens 818). One of
the things that doctors have discovered throughout their research is the wrong
person for placebos. Any patient with negative expectations for treatment
effectiveness, be it a placebo or actual medication, has a much less likely success
rate than those with a drive to be well again. These groups of people are known
as “nocebos.” An individual with a placebo-prone personality will only create a
placebo effect in the presence of positive beliefs and companionship. These
naturally vary from one situation to the next. Even a person that is prone to
the placebo effect can become a nocebo based on the individuals around them.
In the article The Quantification of Placebo Effects Within a General Model of Health
Care Outcomes explains a study completed at three research sites (The
University of Washington Medical Center, the Neurology Center of Fairfax, VA
and the Multiple Sclerosis Association of America, Cherry Hill, NJ). These
three facilities recruited candidates with MS through flyers, mailing and word
of mouth. The three sites were able to take on 117 test subjects. Subjects were
randomly assigned to either the “treatment first” or “placebo first” upon
arrival to the study. At the end of the study the placebo responders “were more
confident that the sham treatment was the active device compared to those who
didn’t respond,” (819 Owens). This study did not only have the expected outcome
but gave placebo theorist exactly what they were looking for. “Positive
expectancy is an essential factor in understanding placebo effects,” (Owens 819). Since placebos are such a questionable
form of treatment it is difficult to know when there is a definite need for
them and when doctors are simply testing their effects. It is general knowledge
that there is no cure for the common cold. Research has shown that placebos
make up 85% of the efficiency of cough medications. The advertising and
packaging alone for these medications begin to get patients brains working
toward feeling better without even taking the medicine. The physiological
effects of cough medicine are initiated by the physical properties of the
medicine such as color, taste, smell and texture. The placebo effect consists
of several components; natural recovery, regression of cough response, effects
of sweetness, voluntary control and effects of the expectancy of the treatment
(Eccles 55).
There have been many positive things
that have come from placebos being used in clinical trial for the common cold.
It used to be that the active ingredient codeine was the standard prescription
for colds. Placebo controlled clinical trial have shown that there is a lack of
efficiency in codeine and placebos have better effects and no addictive side
effects. Many over the counter brands add the bitter taste that the public is
used to tasting to cough syrup to make them feel like they are taking a more
effective treatment. In fact the active codeine in cough syrup had little, if
any specific pharmaceutical effect on the cough.
In order to understand why placebos work
there must be an explanation as to how they work. The placebo effect for a cold
has three major working properties. Ron Eccles explains why placebos have such
a high success rate in cough medicines in his article Importance of Placebo Effect in Cough Clinical Trials The taste of
the cough medicine triggers the mouth to salivate more therefore opening airway
secretion and allowing to irritation in the throat to begin to heal itself. The
bitter sweetness in the medication also numbs the throat to a degree and the
patient is then not thinking about the cold as much. Many ancient cultures used
honey when they were suffering from a cold. The honey would coat their throat
and has medicinal qualities as well. One of the biggest hurdles these people
had to overcome was the quality of medical attention they were able to provide
for their people. Not only did homey help alleviate the coughing symptom, but
it never goes bad. And the biggest argument for a placebo working is the
sensory effect such as taste and smell. Some of the main ingredients in cough
syrups are sugar, honey and spicy substances such as capisum, or bitter
substances such as lemon. These ingredients cause reflex salvation and promote
secretion in the airway (55).
There are very few people in this
world that would complain about placebos in cough medication. When it comes to
the common cold most people are happy to feel better and move on without a
runny nose or sore throat. But what about patients that suffers from more
severe illnesses such as cancer or mental illnesses such as depression?
Many cancer patients are plagued with
rough side effects from chemotherapy such as nausea, vomiting and a major
decline in energy. There is no proven medication on the market to completely
relieve patients of these symptoms but it is not an uncommon practice for
doctors to prescribe a sugar pill to counteract these side effects. Many of
these doctors have to pick and chose who they are going to prescribe these
placebos to because the patient has to be open to feeling better. They must
have a positive outlook for their recovery or the sugar pill will never work.
A doctor by the name of William Potter
was intrigued by placebos and took on the task of testing placebos with mental
illnesses such as depression. The idea behind his research was to better
understand how a placebo could work if the patient was starting their treatment
as more of a nocebo candidate. Potter discovered that the majority of patients
suffering from depression seamed to get healthier simply with doctors empathy
than the pill itself. Since the 1980’s, “two comprehensive analyses of
anti-depressant trials have uncovered a dramatic increase in placebo response”
(33). In a study conducted in Chicago in 2007 almost half the doctors polled
admitted that they knowing prescribed medications that were either completely
ineffective or a very low dose of an effective medication to evoke a placebo
effect (Silberman 41).
Geography also played a major role in
placebos and depression. It is not surprising that Americans had the highest
percentage of taking to antidepressants. The idea that a pill would make one
feel better was not very farfetched for a culture that is surrounded with the
notion of instant gratification. It was found that color also effects the way
people react to a placebo, for example a light blue pill (blue is considered a
stress relieving color) were far more effective that an angry red pill
(Silberman 37).
It is safe to say that placebos
defiantly have a place in the medical field. Placebos work, maybe not for the
population as a whole but there is enough proof that there is no reason they
should not be used as a form of healing. Now for the big question, can the FDA
regulate what placebos are made of and if so how?
As placebos became more commonly
tested in clinical trials the more doctors discovered that the human brain had
the ability to control so much of a person’s healing. From 2001 to 2006 20% of
new drugs being tested were cut after they were put against placebos. Over half
of new medications are dropped because they can’t beat the sugar pills. Steve
Silberman writes, “It’s not that old drugs are getting weaker, it’s as if the
placebo effect is getting stronger” (33).
In the past there have been control
treatments for nondrug interventions such as sham surgery, sham acupuncture or
even sham procedures. So if there is a control for these wouldn’t a placebo
fall under the same category, sham medicine? In the article What’s in Placebos: Who Knows? six
colleagues come together and try to determine an answer. “No substances are
known to be physiologically inert, and no regulations guide placebo
composition” (532).
These colleagues collaborated using four
major medical journals, The New England
Journal of Medicine, JAMA, The Lancet and Annals of Internal Medicine. Within
these journals they discovered 176 eligible studies. In eight of these studies
both pills and injections were used and in one study both injection and
intranasal spray were utilized to administer the placebo. There were a total of
86 studies using pills, 65 studies of injection and 26 studies using other
treatment methods. In all 176 studies the placebo ingredients were seldom
disclosed and were particularly rare of pill based studies. What these
colleagues were able to conclude was that “disclosure of the actual (chemical)
differences between the placebo and test drug is not required,” (Howick et al
534).
As a result of placebos not being
regulated by the FDA there are those of the mindset that the use of placebos
should be few and far between. Vanessa Clifford writes, “The use of placebos in
clinical trials can only be justified ethically when no proven active treatment
is available as a comparison, and conversely when a proven active treatment
exists, the use of placebos is unethical” (361). For Clifford the issue is not
whether or not placebos work but when it is ethical to use them or not. Her
argument is that so many medical patients spend a lot of money for health care
and it is not right for them to be given “medicine.” Even in clinical trials
there is a flaw for testing placebos. Clifford not only believes placebos
should only be used when there are no options for medication but the same goes
for clinical trials as well. She believes placebos should only be introduced in
a clinical trial when there is no proven effect treatment for the condition
(363).
When placebos were first introduced into
the medical field they were used for managing or pleasing a patient when the
diagnosis was uncertain or there was no specific treatment available. Other
doctors and scientist have recently debunked this thesis. In 2001 a Cochrane
review showed that placebos worked their magic best when the patient was in
pain or was heavily open to suggestion.
As one generation of medical
professionals retire and a new younger generation begins to take their places
the way placebos are tested is changing. When placebos were first being studied
the majority of their use was to make patients believe they were being treated
properly. This however was before the days of informed consent and medical
professionals didn’t have to give their patients full disclosure (Clifford
363). Many drug makers are embracing the power of the placebo effect and have
the drive to understand it better. They are learning the mechanics behind it so
they can design trials that help doctors understand the difference between the
medicine itself and the body’s innate ability to heal itself.
It has been proven time and again that
placebos can work. The difficultly that comes with a placebos is how much they
actually work. There are some people who psychiatrically are better suited for
a placebo while others have either made peace with their illness or have no
drive to get better. There is also proof that cough medicine is nothing more
than a placebo. There is no cure for the common cold yet most people have some
sort of cold remedy in their medicine cabinet. It is clear that placebos work,
they make a huge part of western medicine and while the use of them is in need
of some perfecting they do help people for the better not for the worst.
Works
Cited
Clifford V. The
Placebo Mystique: Implications for Clinical Trial Methodology. Journal Of Pediatrics & Child Health [serial
online]. June 2011; 47(6): 361-366. Print.
Eccles R.
Importance of Placebo Effect in Cough Clinical Trials. Lung [serial online]. February 2, 2010; 188:53-61. Print.
Jeremy Howick,
et al. “What’s in Placebos: Who Knows? Analysis of Randomized Controlled
Trials.” Annals of Internal Medicine 153.8
(2010): 532-W. 189 Health Source:
Nursing/Academic Edition. Print.
Owens, Justine
E., and Martha Menard. “The Quantification of Placebo Effects Within a General
Model of Health Care Outcomes.” Journal of Alternative & Complementary
Medicine 17.9 (2011):817-821. Health
Source: Nursing/Academic Edition. Print.
Silberman Steve,
“The Placebo Problem.” 20 Best American
Science Writings 2010 31-44. Print.
No comments:
Post a Comment